Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G. (2000). When does duration matter in judgment and decision making? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 508β529. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G., Prelec, D. (2003). βCoherent arbitrarinessβ: Stable demand curves without stable preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 73β105. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Bazerman, M.H., Loewenstein, G., Blount White, S. (1992). Reversals of preference in allocation decisions: Judging an alternative versus choosing among alternative. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 220β240. Google Scholar | Crossref
Bazerman, M.H., Schroth, H.A., Shah, P.P., Diekmann, K.A., Tenbrunsel, A.E. (1994). The inconsistent role of comparison others and procedural job descriptions: Implications for job acceptance decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 326β352. Google Scholar | Crossref
Bazerman, M.H., Tenbrunsel, A.E., Wade-Benozi, K.A. (1998). Negotiation with yourself and losing: Understanding and managing competing internal preferences. Academic Management Review, 23, 225β241. Google Scholar | Crossref
Brickman, P., Campbell, D.T. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In Appley, M.H. (Ed.), Adaptation level theory: A symposium (pp. 287β302). New York: Academic Press. Google Scholar
Brickman, P., Coates, D., Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 917β927. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Chatterjee, S., Heath, T.B., Min, J. (2009). The susceptibility of mental accounting principles to evaluation mode effects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22, 120β137. Google Scholar | Crossref
Clark, A.E., Frijters, P., Shields, M. (2008). Relative income, happiness and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 95β144. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Cryder, C.E., Mullen, E.E., Loewenstein, G. (2008, November). Wanting versus choosing: A disconnect between what moves us and what we prefer. Paper presented at the Society for Judgment and Decision Making Preconference: Using Human Nature to Improve Human Life, Chicago, IL. Google Scholar
Desvousges, W.H., Johnson, F.R., Dunford, R.W., Boyle, S.P., Wilson, K.N. (1993). Measuring resource damages with contingent valuation. Tests of validity and reliability. In Hausman, J.A. (Ed.), Contingent valuation: A critical assessment (pp. 91β164). Amsterdam: North-Holland. Google Scholar | Crossref
Diener, E., Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective well-being? Social Indicators Research, 57, 119β169. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., Arora, R. (in press). Incomeβs association with judgments of life versus feelings. In Diener, E., Helliwell, J., Kahneman, D. (Eds.), International differences in well-being. New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
Diener, E., Lucas, R.E., Scollon, C.N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist, 61, 305β314. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., Arora, R. (in press). Wealth and happiness across the world: Material prosperity predicts life evaluation, while psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Google Scholar
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Seidlitz, L., Diener, M. (1993). The relationship between income and subjective well-being: Relative or absolute? Social Indicators Research, 28, 195β223. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Dunn, E.W., Wilson, T.D., Gilbert, D.T. (2003). Location, location, location: The misprediction of satisfaction in housing lotteries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1421β1432. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI
Easterlin, R.A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? In David, P.A., Reder, M.W. (Eds.), Nations and households in economic growth: Essays in honor of Moses Abramovitz (pp. 89β125). New York: Academic Press. Google Scholar
Easterlin, R.A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27, 35β47. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison. Human Relations, 7, 117β140. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI
Fredrick, S., Loewenstein, G., OβDonoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351β401. Google Scholar | Crossref
Fredrickson, B.L., Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 45β55. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Gilbert, D.T., Gill, M.J., Wilson, T.D. (2002). The future is now: Temporal correction in affective forecasting. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 430β444. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Gilbert, D.T., Pinel, E.C., Wilson, T.D., Blumberg, S.J., Wheatley, T. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617β638. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Gonzalez, R., Wu, G. (1999). On the shape of the probability weighting function. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 129β166. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Gonzalez-Vallejo, C., Moran, E. (2001). The evaluability hypothesis revisited: Joint and separate evaluation preference reversal as a function of attribute importance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 216β233. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row. Google Scholar
Hsee, C.K. (1996). The evaluability hypothesis: An explanation for preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of alternatives. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 67, 247β257. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Hsee, C.K. (1998). Less is better: When low-value options are valued more highly than high-value options. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11, 107β121. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Hsee, C.K., Blount, S., Loewenstein, G., Bazerman, M. (1999). Preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of options: A review and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 576β590. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Hsee, C.K., Rottenstreich, Y. (2004). Music, pandas, and muggers: On the affective psychology of value. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 23β30. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Hsee, C.K., Shen, L. (2009). Taxi versus horse: What affects consumer price sensitivity and when price-setters mispredict it? Unpublished manuscript. Google Scholar
Hsee, C.K., Yang, Y., Li, N., Shen, L. (2009). Wealth, warmth, and well-being: Whether happiness is relative or absolute depends on whether it is about money, acquisition, or consumption. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 396β409. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI
Hsee, C.K., Zhang, J. (2004). Distinction bias: Misprediction and mischoice due to joint evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 680β695. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Irwin, J.R., Slovic, P., Lichenstein, S., McClelland, G. (1993). Preference reversals and the measurement of environmental values. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 6, 5β18. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Kahneman, D. (2008). The sad tale of the aspiration treadmill. Retrieved May 21, 2009, from http://www.edge.org/q2008/q08_17.html#kahneman Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Fredrickson, B.L., Schreiber, C.A., Redelmeier, D.A. (1993). When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science, 4, 401β405. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI
Kahneman, D., Ritov, I. (1994). Determinants of stated willingness to pay for public goods: A study in the headline method. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 9, 5β38. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Kahneman, D., Snell, J. (1992). Predicting a changing taste: Do people know what they will like? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 5, 187β200. Google Scholar | Crossref
Kahneman, D., Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263β292. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Kluger, A.N., Stephan, E., Ganzach, Y., Hershkovitz, M. (2004). The effect of regulatory focus on the shape of probability-weighting function: Evidence from a cross-modality matching method. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95, 20β39. Google Scholar | Crossref
Kogut, T., Ritov, I. (2005). The singularity effect of identified victims in separate and joint evaluations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97, 106β116. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Larrick, R.P. (2004). Debiasing. In Koehler, D.J., Harvey, N. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 316β337). Oxford, United Kingdom: Blackwell. Google Scholar | Crossref
List, J. (2002). Preference reversals of a different kind: The βmore is lessβ phenomenon. American Economic Review, 92, 1636β1643. Google Scholar | Crossref
Loewenstein, G., OβDonoghue, T., Rabin, M. (2003). Projection bias in predicting future utility. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 1209β1248. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Loewenthal, D. (1993). Reversals of preference in candidate choice. Unpublished manuscript, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA. Google Scholar
Moore, D.A. (1999). Order effects in preference judgments: Evidence for context dependence in the generation of preferences. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 146β165. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Morewedge, C.K., Kassam, K.S., Hsee, C.K., Caruso, E.M. (2009). Duration sensitivity depends on stimulus familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 177β186. Google Scholar | Crossref | Medline | ISI
Paharia, N., Kassam, K.S., Greene, J.D., Bazerman, M.H. (2009). Dirty work, clean hands: The moral psychology of indirect agency. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 134β141. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Read, D. (2004). Intertemporal choice. In Koehler, D.J., Harvey, N. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 424β443). Oxford, United Kingdom: Blackwell. Google Scholar | Crossref
Read, D., Frederick, S., Orsel, B., Rahman, J. (2005). Four scores and seven years from now: The date/delay effect in temporal discounting. Management Science, 51, 1326β1335. Google Scholar | Crossref
Rottenstreich, Y., Hsee, C.K. (2001). Money, kisses, and electronic shocks: On the affective psychology of risk. Psychological Science, 12, 185β190. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals
Schkade, D., Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make people happy? A focusing illusion in judgments of life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 9, 340β346. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI
Schmeltzer, C., Caverni, J., Warglien, M. (2004). How does preference reversal appear and disappear? Effects of the evaluation mode. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 395β408. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Soman, D., Ainslie, G., Frederick, S., Xiuping, L., Moreau, P., Mitchell, A. (2005). The psychology of intertemporal discounting: Why are distant events valued more than proximal events? Marketing Letters, 347β360. Google Scholar | Crossref
Stevenson, B., Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin paradox. Bookings Papers on Economic Activity, 1β87. Google Scholar
Tehila, K., Ritov, I. (2005). The singularity effect of identified victims in separate and joint evaluations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 97, 106β116. Google Scholar | Crossref
Van Boven, L., Loewenstein, G., Dunning, D. (2005). The illusion of courage in social predictions: Underestimating the impact of fear of embarrassment on other people. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96, 130β141. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Veenhoven, R. (1991). Is happiness relative? Social Indicators Research, 24, 1β34. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Willemsen, M.C., Keren, G. (2004). The role of negative features in joint and separate evaluation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17, 313β329. Google Scholar | Crossref
Wilson, T.D., Gilbert, D. (2003). Affective forecasting. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 345β411. Google Scholar | Crossref | ISI
Yeung, C., Soman, D. (2005). Attribute evaluability and the range effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 363β369. Google Scholar | Crossref
Yeung, C., Soman, D. (2007). The duration heuristic. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 315β326. Google Scholar | Crossref
Zhang, J., Hsee, C.K. (2009). Distinction bias: The moderating role of attribute evaluability. Unpublished manuscript. Google Scholar
Zikmund-Fisher, B.J., Fagerlin, A., Ubel, P.A. (2004). βIs 28% good or bad?β Evaluability and preference reversals in health care decisions. Medical Decision Making, 24, 142β148. Google Scholar | SAGE Journals | ISI